Primate Info Net Banner Wisconsin PRC Logo
PIN Home > About the Primates > Primate Factsheets > Nomascus leucogenys

White-cheeked gibbon
Nomascus leucogenys

SOCIAL ORGANIZATION AND BEHAVIOR

Very little is known about the behavioral ecology of white-cheeked gibbons. Most information comes from the very small Chinese population and details about the Laotian and Vietnamese populations are based on anecdotal field observations (Geissmann et al. 2000). However, the available data suggest that the life history characteristics of the white-cheeked gibbon are similar to other gibbons (Geissmann et al. 2000). Historically, gibbons were thought to live only in monogamous social groups that include an adult male and female and their immature offspring. Recent research has shed light on the social structure of gibbons, though, and while many form long-term pair bonds, extra-pair copulations may occur (Palombit 1994; Reichard 1995). A gibbon family is territorial and defends its territory with regular morning songs performed by the breeding male and female (Leighton 1987). Groups have, on average, four individuals and usually include an adult male and female, one infant and one juvenile, although the group may also include one adolescent and one subadult (Leighton 1987). There is a high degree of social and behavioral equality between adult males and females and codominance is exhibited (Leighton 1987; Geissmann et al. 2000). Because of this egalitarian social atmosphere and no competition between males for access to females, white-cheeked gibbons are not sexually dimorphic.

Nomascus leucogenys
Photo: Bertrand L. Deputte

Gibbons defend their territories both vocally and physically. The primary means by which adult males and females defend their territory is by ritualized morning duets. The morning duet occurs at dawn and is a long, loud, energetically expensive song in which the female and male sing individual notes in specific patterns. The duet lasts between 10 and 13 minutes but is longer during the rainy season and shorter during the dry season (Geissmann et al. 2000). This seasonal variation in song bout may be related to energy expenditure and food availability. During the rainy season, when fruit is widely available and foraging time is shorter, more time and energy can be allocated for singing whereas during the dry season, foraging time increases and more energy is expended on finding food rather than territory defense (Geissmann et al. 2000). The song is initiated by either sex but is always ended by the adult male (Schilling 1984). Females begin singing long notes of increasing frequency with note durations and intervals between notes continuously decreasing and reaching a climax of short barks that eventually trail off (Geissmann et al. 2000). Males sing three different notes that include boom notes, staccato notes, and high frequency modulated notes. During his part of the duet, a male continuously cycles through these three notes and his song is accompanied by locomotory displays including violent brachiation, branch shaking, or prancing (Deputte 1982; Geissmann et al. 2000). The other mechanisms by which gibbons defend their territories include aggression and physical contests. Intergroup encounters may include chasing, fighting, and loud-call displays as well (Leighton 1987). Reports of facial scars, broken canines, and open wounds have been documented in some but not all gibbon species and it is usually males that are afflicted (Leighton 1987). Females are usually involved in call displays, both morning duets and other calls during border disputes, while adult, subadult, and adolescent males of a gibbon group physically defend their territory from intruders or full takeovers (Leighton 1987).

Because of the monogamous social structure, both males and females leave their natal groups when they reach sexual maturity, and transfer is hastened by aggressive encounters between the subadult gibbon and its same-sexed parent (Leighton 1987; Macdonald 2001). Gibbon parents often become antagonistic towards their subadult offspring and evict them from the family group to decrease the possibility of inbreeding (Leighton 1987). Though young gibbons attempt to settle near their natal territories, the availability of vacant or appropriate habitat is a limiting factor. Females are more likely to remain close to their natal territories than males, and vagabond young males seek out females. Once paired, a gibbon couple then settles in an appropriate habitat and begins to defend it as their own territory (Leighton 1987).

REPRODUCTION

Limited data are available on reproductive parameters in both wild and captive white-cheeked gibbon. Sexual maturity occurs between six and eight years of age but captive white-cheeked gibbons have bred as early as four years-old (Geissmann 1991). With sexual maturity comes a change in pelage color of female white-cheeked gibbons from their black juvenile coat to the characteristic yellow or buffy coat of adults. Males remain black but develop the white cheek patches as they reach sexual maturity (Geissmann et al. 2000). The menstrual cycle of female gibbons lasts, on average, 21.6 days (between 12 and 27 days) and is accompanied by some genital swelling during estrus, though it is not as pronounced as that seen in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) and bonobos (Pan paniscus) (Nadler et al. 1993; Lukas et al. 2002). During estrus, female gibbons entice males to mate with quick, jerking upper-body movements known as bobbing (Lukas et al. 2002). First parturition occurs between six and eight years of age and gibbons exhibit an interbirth interval of two to three years. A mating pair of gibbons will produce an average of five to six offspring over their reproductive lifetimes (Leighton 1987; Geissmann 1991; Macdonald 2001). There is no conclusive evidence for birth seasonality in white-cheeked gibbons, though some species of gibbon exhibit mild birth peaks at the end of the rainy season (Leighton 1987).

PARENTAL CARE

Though white-cheeked gibbons are monogamous and males have high paternity certainty, the majority of parental care is the responsibility of the female gibbon (Varsik 2001). Gestation lasts about seven months (Lukas et al. 2002). Infancy lasts from birth to about two years, at which time the infant is weaned and enters the juvenile period (Leighton 1987; Macdonald 2001). Juvenile gibbons move independently of their mothers and this period lasts from two to four years (Leighton 1987). The period of adolescence is from about four to six years of age in which the young gibbon is not yet adult-sized but is moving and sleeping independently from its mother and beginning to reach sexual maturity. According to Leighton (1987), young gibbons were not observed to play on a daily basis, this may be due to the small social groups and lack of play partners.

COMMUNICATION

Nomascus leucogenys
Photo: Bertrand L. Deputte

The territorial long call duets of white-cheeked gibbons serve an important function by communicating location and area of occupancy to other gibbon groups. The development of this duet begins during the juvenile period when young gibbons practice the songs and displays that they will one day perform as adults in their own territories (Deputte 1982). Juvenile males and females mimic portions of an adult female's song while she is performing her great call. As they mature, adolescent females begin to hone their singing skills and eventually perfect the song of the adult female white-cheeked gibbon. Young males will eventually switch the type of call they give (usually coinciding with sexual maturity) and begin to sing the chorus of an adult male as they are gradually peripheralized from the family group (Deputte 1982; Geissmann et al. 2001).

LISTEN TO VOCALIZATIONS

A form of visual communication in white-cheeked gibbons is the sexual dichromism exhibited by adult males and females. Coat color is an intraspecies cue that conveys information about age and reproductive status, and it may also function in mate choice (Gerald 2003). The differences in coat color and markings are important, for example, to distinguish sexually mature males from subadult males. Perhaps this cue is useful for dispersing females looking for a potential mate; they can quickly identify if males are mature enough to be potential mates (Gerald 2003).

SPECIAL NOTES

Other reasons that gibbons are separated from the great apes include the differences in intelligence and tool-using behaviors. Unlike chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes), bonobos (Pan paniscus), gorillas (Gorilla) and orangutans (Pongo), gibbons do not use tools in the wild and do not score as well on cognitive tests in captivity (Rumbaugh & Washburn 2003). In captivity, gibbons have been recorded using objects in their enclosures as tools, but short of dropping branches on human observers that follow them, they have not been seen using tools in the wild. On intelligence tests they do not generally perform as well as great apes but generally do better than monkeys (Rumbaugh & Washburn 2003). In recent tests, though, gibbons moderately scored higher in a cognitive task than chimpanzees and capuchins (Cebus) (Cuningham et al. in press).

Content last modified: December 1, 2010

Written by Kristina Cawthon Lang. Reviewed by Alan Mootnick.

Cite this page as:
Cawthon Lang KA. 2010 December 1. Primate Factsheets: White-cheeked gibbon (Nomascus leucogenys) Behavior . <http://pin.primate.wisc.edu/factsheets/entry/white-cheeked_gibbon/behav>. Accessed 2014 October 24.